|
||
|
EDITOR’S NOTE: The following article was written for
Parenting for Excellence (Volume I, No. 4), an educational newsletter
published by The Stelle Group. Through its subject is “mothering” and culture,
we believe it is important information for all adults. The
mothering children receive defines the world for them, gives them their
identity, and sets the pattern for their relationships with other human
beings. The
kind of mothering a generation of children receives is the greatest single
determinant of the quality of the culture that generation produces as adults. MOTHERING BY MOTHERS — AND OTHERS Optimally
the mothering a child receives is from that child’s biological mother, begins
before birth, and is consistently attuned to that
particular child. Sometimes the mothering function is
performed by the father or by someone else other than the biological
mother. And sometimes this alternative mothering, if
consistently, lovingly present during the first several years after birth, is
effective in facilitating the child’s healthy development. The
fathering function is important—extremely important in fact. It is, however,
a different function. MOTHERING DETERMINES THE QUALITY OF CULTURE Perhaps
in some history class somewhere you heard the story
too—about the Chinese mothers. Historians were puzzled over how Those
generations of Chinese mothers clearly demonstrated the power mothering has
to affect history, but the lesson seems to have gotten lost. Our culture’s
truism, “The hand that rocks the cradle rules the world,” is
generally regarded as merely a nice bit of sentiment. So is Psychoanalyst
and historian Lloyd deMause, in The History of Childhood, concluded
that the central force for change in history is alteration of the personality
of generations of children due to changes in parenting modes. A supporter of this concept, historian Glenn Davis, studied the
differing patterns of parenting used in rearing four American presidents and
became convinced that those parenting patterns were predictive of major
national social thrusts associated with the four men! Another
strong stand for the significance of mothering is found
in Selma Fraiberg’s book Every Child’s Birthright: In Defense of
Mothering. In it, a statement with surprisingly practical ramifications is made. Economist Harold Shapiro, chairman of the Department
of Economics at the University of Michigan, is reported as stating that
optimal development in a generation of a nation’s children is reflected in
future productivity in that nation’s labor market! And
most of the rest of Fraiberg’s book is a carefully supported explanation of
how this optimal development in childhood is dependent upon optimal exercise
of the mothering function—or proper use, in other words, of that
cradle-rocking hand. If Chinese mothers determine what cultural consciousness
pervades their country, if the parenting of presidents shows up later in
national social movements, and if the quality of mothering affects a
nation’s future productivity, then perhaps that old truism needs only slight
amendment to become a simple cause-and-effect statement: “The hand that rocks
the cradle rules the world of the next generation.” And perhaps MOTHERING SETS THE PATTERN FOR INDIVIDUAL DEVELOPMENT Although
historians and social scientists have carefully traced how patterns of
parenting affect society, their conclusions are eventually rooted in what
psychologists have studied even more thoroughly—the impact of mothering on
individual development. The effects of early mothering, and its lack, have
been studied at length by Margaret Mabler, John Bowlby, and Rene Spitz, among others. The highly respected work of all
three shows how crucial a reliable mothering presence is to every aspect of a
child’s growth. Their work also indicates what many other studies show as
well, that that presence alone is not enough; the way a mother is
present to her child affects the child’s overall development—sets patterns
for all future growth. For
example, in The Roots of Love, Helene Arnstein reports that Drs. Sylvia
Brody and Sidney Axelrod conducted a fairly simple-sounding
study of how mothers feed their babies. Films were taken
of feeding sessions when each baby was six weeks, six months, and twelve
months old. Analysis of the films and tests of the babies revealed amazing
significance in this very mundane process. Some babies experienced a pattern
of happy feeding times, while other infants usually exhibited unhappiness
during feedings. At the end of a year, those babies who had had generally
pleasurable mealtime experiences showed, “a much greater capacity to wait, to
concentrate, to learn, to solve problems, and to anticipate and expect
pleasure from people and things than did the babies who had experienced
anxieties and tensions during their feedings.” When
these same children were studied at the time they
entered grade school, their intellectual, emotional, and social development still
mirrored the patterns of interaction established between mother and child
during that first year of life! Well,
have we found them, then? Are those much sought
basic requirements for intelligence and mental health as simple as relaxed
mealtimes and a reliable mothering presence? We
are learning more each year about how to help children maximize their
potential. How the brain works, what the essential stages of growth are, and
how the physical and mental relate, these are only a few of the factors in
child development that are being studied by scientists and put to use by
parents in this generation. The details are intriguing and valuable. But probably the most powerful effect of the research is
its recurring theme: that the patterns for how children will actualize their
potential are intricately and closely bound up in the kind of mothering they experienced.
And the kind of mothering given a generation of
children provides the shape of the world those children fashion when they
become adults. _________________________________ The
Stelle Group has accepted The Editor |
|
|
|